Button to scroll to the top of the page.

News

From the College of Natural Sciences
Font size: +

Dominant Individuals are the Least Influential, Study Finds

Dominant Individuals are the Least Influential, Study Finds
A new study of cichlid fish behavior shows that dominant individuals can influence a group through force, but passive individuals are far better at bringing a group to consensus. Photo credit: The Jordan Lab, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior.

Being the strongest, biggest and most aggressive individual in a group might make you dominant, but it doesn't mean you make all the decisions.

A new study of fish behavior published in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences shows that dominant individuals can influence a group through force, but passive individuals are far better at bringing a group to consensus. The study, published by an international team from the Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, the University of Konstanz and the University of Texas at Austin, overturns assumptions that dominant individuals also have the greatest influence on their groups, and sheds light on the potential of domineering individuals to obstruct effective communication in organizations.

"The same traits that make you powerful in one context can actively reduce your influence in others, especially contexts in which individuals are free to choose who to follow," said senior author Alex Jordan, a group leader at the Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior. "Dominant individuals can force their will on the group by being pushy, but that also makes them socially aversive. When it comes to bringing peers to consensus during more sophisticated tasks, it is the least aggressive individuals that exert the greatest influence. Our results illustrate that although domineering individuals most often ascend to positions of power, they can in fact create the least effective influence structures at the same time."

To disentangle the effects of dominance and influence, the researchers studied groups of a social cichlid fish, Astatotilpia burtoni.

"This species forms groups with strict social hierarchies, in which dominant males control resources, territory, and space," said Mariana Rodriguez-Santiago, co-first author on the study and a doctoral student in the lab of co-corresponding author Hans Hofmann at UT Austin. "We ask if the colorful dominant males, which are aggressive, central in their social networks, and control resources, are most influential? Or if drab subordinate males wield the greatest influence, despite being passive, non-territorial, and having little or no control over resources."

The researchers separated the effects of social dominance from social influence by examining how information flows between either dominant or subordinate males and their groups in two different contexts: routine social behavior, or a more complex social learning task. In the more complex social learning task, dominant or subordinate male fish were trained that a certain colored light on one side of the tank meant food would soon arrive at that location. These "informed" individuals were then placed into new groups of uninformed individuals and researchers asked which group—those with informed dominant or subordinate males—more quickly learned to associate a colored light with food.

The researchers observed the movement of the fish, and found that in routine social interactions the dominant males exerted the greatest influence by chasing and pushing the group around. But in the more complex task, where influence was not forced on the group, but rather individuals had a choice about who to follow, it was subordinate males who wielded the greatest influence in their social groups. In groups with a subordinate male as demonstrator, fish quickly came to a consensus about which light to follow, moving together as a coherent unit to succeed in the task. With a dominant male as the informant, groups were far slower to reach consensus, if they did at all.

By using additional machine-learning based animal tracking, employing cutting edge techniques developed in the gaming and graphics industries, researchers were able to break down the behavioral differences between dominant and subordinate males: dominant males were central in behavioral social networks (they frequently interacted with others) but they occupied peripheral locations in spatial networks (they were avoided by others). The technology provided insights never before available, revealing the mechanisms of influence as well as the outcome.

"By capturing behavioral data that are impossible to be measured with the naked eye, our automated tracking methods revealed that it was not the difference in social position between dominant and subordinate per se, but rather in the way they moved and interacted with others," said co-first author Paul Nührenberg, a doctoral student at the Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behavior, a Cluster of Excellence at the University of Konstanz. "These behavioral differences lead directly to differences in social influence."

This result touches on the evolution of animal societies as well as leadership structures in organizations. "In many societies, whether animal or human, individuals in positions of power all possess a similar suite of traits, which are aggression, intimidation and coercion," said Jordan. "But effective communication requires the presence of a diversity of voices, not just the loudest. Our results from a natural system show that allowing alternative pathways to positions of power may be useful in creating stronger advisory, governmental and educational structures."

This post was adapted from a press release by the University of Konstanz.

Power of DNA to Store Information Gets an Upgrade
Discovery about Hep C Drug Offers Insights for Cor...

Comments

 
No comments made yet. Be the first to submit a comment
Already Registered? Login Here
Guest
Wednesday, 16 October 2024

Captcha Image